False-Belief Understanding Measure

From GM-RKB
(Redirected from false-belief understanding)
Jump to navigation Jump to search

A False-Belief Understanding Measure is a cognitive measure that is calculated by testing an individual's ability to understand that others can hold false beliefs.



References

2023

  • chat
    • False-belief understanding refers to the ability to recognize that others may hold beliefs or perspectives that are different from one's own and that these beliefs may not be accurate or true. It is a crucial aspect of social cognition and theory of mind, which is the capacity to understand and interpret the mental states of others, such as their desires, emotions, intentions, and beliefs.

      The development of false-belief understanding typically occurs during early childhood, around the ages of 4 to 5, and is considered an important milestone in a child's cognitive and social development. It enables children to engage in more complex social interactions, predict the behavior of others, and develop empathy.

      A classic experiment to test false-belief understanding is the Sally-Anne task, which involves two dolls named Sally and Anne. In this task, a child watches a scenario where Sally places a toy in a specific location and then leaves the room. While Sally is gone, Anne moves the toy to a different location. When she returns, the child is then asked where Sally will look for the toy. If the child understands false belief, they will correctly predict that Sally will look for the toy in the original location, as she is unaware that Anne moved it. This demonstrates the child's ability to understand that Sally holds a belief different from their own and that this belief is false.

2023

  • (Wikipedia, 2023) ⇒ https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/theory_of_mind#False-belief_task Retrieved:2023-3-20.
    • One of the most important milestones in theory of mind development is the ability to attribute false belief: in other words, to understand that other people can believe things which are not true. To do this, it is suggested, one must understand how knowledge is formed, that people's beliefs are based on their knowledge, that mental states can differ from reality, and that people's behavior can be predicted by their mental states. Numerous versions of false-belief task have been developed, based on the initial task created by Wimmer and Perner (1983).[1]

      In the most common version of the false-belief task (often called the "'Sally-Anne' test" or "'Sally-Anne' task"), children are told or shown a story involving two dolls, Sally and Anne, who have a basket and a box, respectively. Sally also has a marble, which she places into her basket, and then leaves the room. While she is out of the room, Anne takes the marble from the basket and puts it into the box. Sally returns, and the child is then asked where Sally will look for the marble. The child passes the task if she answers that Sally will look in the basket, where Sally put the marble; the child fails the task if she answers that Sally will look in the box, where the child knows the marble is hidden, even though Sally cannot know this, since she did not see it hidden there. To pass the task, the child must be able to understand that another's mental representation of the situation is different from their own, and the child must be able to predict behavior based on that understanding.[2]

      Another example depicts a boy who leaves chocolate on a shelf and then leaves the room. His mother puts it in the fridge. To pass the task, the child must understand that the boy, upon returning, holds the false belief that his chocolate is still on the shelf.[3]

      The results of research using false-belief tasks have been fairly consistent: most typically developing children are able to pass the tasks from around age four.[4] Notably, while most children, including those with Down syndrome, are able to pass this test, in one study, 80% of children diagnosed with autism were unable to do so.[5]

      Adults may also struggle with false beliefs, for instance when they show hindsight bias—"the inclination to see events that have already happened as being more predictable than they were before they took place."[6] In one experiment, adult subjects who were asked for an independent assessment were unable to disregard information on actual outcome. Also in experiments with complicated situations, when assessing others' thinking, adults can fail to correctly disregard certain information that they have been given.[3]

  1. Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103-128. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5.
  2. O'Brien, K., Slaughter, V., & Peterson, C. C. (2011). Sibling influences on theory of mind development for children with ASD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(6), 713-719. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02389.x
  3. 3.0 3.1 Mitchell, P. (2011). Acquiring a theory of mind. In A. Slater & J. G. Bremner (Eds.), An introduction to developmental psychology (3rd ed., pp. 381–406). John Wiley & Sons Inc.
  4. Roessler, Johannes (2013). When the Wrong Answer Makes Perfect Sense - How the Beliefs of Children Interact With Their Understanding of Competition, Goals and the Intention of Others. University of Warwick Knowledge Centre. Archived from the original on 3 December 2013. Retrieved 15 August 2013.
  5. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a "theory of mind"?. Cognition, 21(1), 37–46. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8.
  6. Mitchell, P. (2011). “Acquiring a Theory of Mind". In Alan Slater, & Gavin Bremner (eds.) An Introduction to Developmental Psychology: Second Edition, BPS Blackwell. page 371

2010

2009